Untangling the Social and Empathy Dynamics of Autism

Psychological research is exploring this complex and increasingly common neurodevelopmental condition through the lens of neurodiversity.

Illustration by Jeannie Phan
January 31, 2022 By Sharon Aschaiek
View PDF version

Over the last two decades, autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has become the fastest-growing developmental disability worldwide. In 2020, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention in the United States identified one in 54 children as having ASD, a neurological condition causing social and communication delays and restricted, repetitive behaviours. The diagnosis rate has tripled since 2000, when it was one in 150, and grown more than tenfold since the 1990s, when it was one in 1,000. This steep rise in prevalence has transformed autism into a public health crisis requiring more attention from government leaders, health professionals and educators. It has also fuelled significantly more research into autism, particularly its contributing factors—genetic mutations, poorly functioning gut bacteria and, possibly, environmental pollution.

Share this article

But Yonat Rum and Anat Perry are asking different questions about autism, ones that consider the perspectives, abilities and life experiences of those on the autism spectrum. More specifically, these psychology researchers at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem (HUJI) approach autism through the lens of neurodiversity, an increasingly accepted theoretical perspective that views autism not as a problem to be solved, but as the result of natural variations in the human brain. This approach is leading to greater recognition of the strengths of autistic individuals, and allowing for the development of policies and practices to include them in society.

“When I started researching autism, I found that most studies focus on the deficits or impairments of people on the spectrum. But there are many insights we can gain about the positive aspects of autism...”
Yonat Rum

The Sibling Factor in Socialization

Rum’s interest in autism stems from her education and work experiences in special education, and in 2013, it became the subject of her PhD research, which she completed as an Azrieli Graduate Studies Fellow at Tel Aviv University. She had noticed a dearth of knowledge in the field about how the social communication abilities of children with autism may be shaped by interactions with their neurotypical (NT) siblings. Rum decided to tackle the subject for her doctoral dissertation. Her research included three studies that examined the relationships and interactions between children with ASD and their older NT sibling. She shared her findings in a co-authored study that was published last November in the International Journal of Behavioral Development: “Prosocial behaviors of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) during interactions with their typically developing siblings.”

For one observational, naturalistic study, Rum watched and video recorded 28 sibling pairs, each including a child with ASD and an older NT sibling, engaging in free play together at home. Her goals were to assess the level of collaboration in these interactions and detect the characteristics of prosocial behaviours, play-related behaviours, fighting, discourse and imitation. Subsequent coding and analysis of these interactions using a frame-by-frame computerized analytic tool revealed collaborative play to be present in 78 per cent of the sibling pairs. Prosocial behaviours were identified as the second most frequent type of behaviour observed, after play-related behaviours. The number of prosocial behaviours displayed by the ASD sibling was closely associated with the number of prosocial behaviours displayed by the NT sibling.

“We concluded that a child with autism benefits from the relationship with an NT sibling, because it provides a social role model and an opportunity to practise social behaviours in a generally collaborative and accepting environment,” says Rum, who is currently conducting postdoctoral research at the Autism Research Centre at the University of Cambridge under renowned autism expert Simon Baron-Cohen. “Often, children with autism are busy participating in therapies instructed by adults, but this study shows there is also value in these natural interactions of free play at home with their siblings.”

Psychology researcher Yonat Rum has identified distinct social communication benefits that arise for children with autism when interacting with their neurotypical siblings.

Photograph by Shauli Lendner

The Literature on Autism and Empathy

Recently, Rum has shifted her attention to how empathy presents in adults with autism. This has led to research collaborations with Azrieli Faculty Fellow Anat Perry, founder and director of the Social Cognitive Neuroscience Lab at HUJI. Perry and her team study the social, cognitive and neural aspects of social behaviours in healthy and clinical populations in two main areas: neural mechanisms that enable empathy, and interpersonal distance in social interactions. Rum’s and Perry’s overlapping research interests led them to partner on “Empathic Accuracy in Clinical Populations” (Frontiers in Psychiatry, June 2020), a review of 34 peer-reviewed studies, published between 1997 and 2019, on empathic accuracy—the ability to accurately judge others’ thoughts and feelings—in individuals with autism or another neurological or behavioural disorder.

The overall findings from the eight studies involving autism suggest that empathic accuracy is reduced in individuals with ASD. However, Rum and Perry note a number of factors that complicate the validity of this finding. To start with, the body of research is too small to support broad conclusions about empathic accuracy and autism. Also, females were significantly underrepresented in these studies, which makes it impossible to generalize the results among the wider population. Another mitigating factor is that empathic accuracy can be more difficult for individuals with ASD due to challenges with attention, executive functioning and motor skills. Rum and Perry say it is unclear to what extent this was taken into account during the development of the eight studies. Altogether, they say, these factors reflect the need for more research into autism and empathic accuracy that considers these variables.

To discern how we understand other people’s emotions, Perry has developed a naturalistic yet controlled empathic accuracy paradigm. She applied this approach in “The contribution of linguistic and visual cues to physiological synchrony and empathic accuracy” (Cortex, November 2020). The goal of this study, which involved neurotypical participants, was to better understand the behavioural and physiological dynamics of two main aspects of empathy: mentalizing, which is understanding another person’s emotional state through context, and experience sharing, which is resonating with another person’s emotions.

Hebrew University of Jerusalem psychology researcher Anat Perry is using the empathic accuracy paradigm she developed to examine how we come to understand the emotions of others.

Photograph by Boaz Perlstein

The study consisted of two phases. The first involved video-recording 28 male and female individuals—the “targets”—conducting an empathic accuracy task: telling an emotional story about their lives. The stories focused on topics such as the illness of a family member, a romantic breakup or a conflict with a friend. During their storytelling, each target’s heart rate was tracked with an electrocardiogram. The targets were then asked to watch their own story and continuously rate their affective valence (how positive or negative they felt) when telling the story. They also reported the specific emotions they felt, and rated the intensity of each on a scale from one (not at all) to nine (a lot).

In the second phase, the same stories were presented to 72 new participant “observers” in one of three ways: video with audio, video only or audio only. The observers each watched and/or listened to nine stories and were asked to continuously rate each target’s affective valence with the same measure previously used by the targets. Following each story, they were asked to specify the emotions they inferred that the target had felt. Their heart rates were recorded throughout the experiment. Lastly, all of the observers completed two well-established empathy questionnaires.

Perry and her research team had hypothesized that empathic accuracy would be greater when audio was present, and that physiological synchrony, a proxy for experience sharing, would be greater with the video-only versions. Their key findings revealed that, indeed, when there were audio cues, either alone or with video, observers showed high empathic accuracy, meaning they recognized the targets’ emotions well. This may be because words are so influential that when they are present, one relies mostly on them to make inferences. When visual cues were presented alone, empathic accuracy scores were much lower but still better than chance. However, with visual cues only, heart rate synchrony was highest, meaning that observers’ heart rates were in sync with those of the targets. Without words, one needs to interpret emotions from body language alone, which may elevate the importance of physiological synchrony as an indicator of empathy.

“Empathy is a mutual process that requires understanding by both sides.”
Echoes Rum

This study was our first using this naturalistic empathic accuracy paradigm.

“We can now leverage this paradigm to study how different information channels contribute to or hinder empathy in diverse populations, from people with attention deficits to stroke patients and, of course, in people on the autism spectrum,” says Perry, whose Azrieli Fellowship is funding one of her research programs in this area, called “Empathic accuracy and its dysfunction in autism: neurobehavioural characterization and potential modulation.”

Perry and Rum will now conduct a new study that uses the same format but involves individuals with autism, as both observers and targets. Their investigations will include a specific focus on females with ASD, an understudied clinical population that is just starting to be acknowledged by the scientific community.

“Yonat helped me realize that, yes, people with ASD may have deficits in understanding others, but no one studies how well we understand them,” Perry says. “British sociologist Damian Milton refers to this as the ‘double empathy problem’—empathy involves reciprocity between two social actors. Both sides need to understand each other.”

Echoes Rum: “Empathy is a mutual process that requires understanding by both sides. If a neurotypical person observes an autistic person, how much will they empathize? That’s what we want to find out.”

Have information and updates about the Azrieli Fellows Program sent directly to your inbox.

Subscribe to our newsletter

* indicates required

All fields are mandatory.

Top